Advertisement
Advertisement
Business Insider
Get more with myNEWS
A personalised news feed of stories that matter to you
Learn more
Photo: Darren Weaver

A new study reveals that certain drugs may make you smarter than we thought

Modafinil and methylphenidate seem to be effective cognitive enhancers

By Kevin Loria

It’s both appealing and disturbing to think that taking a pill could boost someone’s brainpower to make them “smarter.”

Films like “Limitless” and “Lucy” create fantasies of cognition-enhancing drugs that practically provide superpowers. In the real world, college students and finance workers have been known to cram while hopped up on medications like Adderall and Ritalin, pharmaceuticals normally used to treat attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).

But data has historically been mixed when researchers investigated the impact of those drugs on mental capacity. Some studies have found so-called “smart drugs” to have cognitive benefits, but others have shown they don’t make a difference for many people, especially those of average or above-average intelligence.

A new study that tested the effects of various cognitive enhancers on chess players might help clear up that mystery. Findings show that the drugs modafinil (commonly known as Provigil) and methyphenidate (frequently known as Ritalin or Concerta) can both improve the performance of chess players — those studied were better at performing the mental calculations needed to play chess after taking the drugs. However the study also revealed some of the limitations of these effects, since the drugs’ enhancement came at a cost to time management, especially for players who already struggled to play quickly.

Better, but slower, chess players

The researchers selected 40 above-average chess players for the study. All were men, with an average age of 37 and an average IQ of 127.7. The group had an average Elo rating (a rating system for chess skill) in the 1670s, which puts them significantly above the novice level though not quite in the expert or master categories. 

In their first analysis, they found that the players on caffeine, modafinil, or methylphenidate did about 6-8% better than those on placebos — enough to see a trend but not enough to be statistically significant (to rule out the possibility, in other words, that their improved results were due to chance).

But in taking a closer look at the data, the researchers found that players spent significantly more time thinking about each move they made after taking the drugs. This seems to have made them worse at time management, especially the players who already struggled with timed games.

At the same time, the players seemed to be making better moves during that time. “This suggests that neuro-enhancers ... improve the players’ ability or willingness to spend more time on a decision and hence to perform more thorough calculations,” the authors wrote.

The researchers then looked at the data in two other ways: they discounted the games lost because time ran out (for all players), and also removed the five players who struggled most with time management from the data set. These analyses significantly improved the results for modafinil and methylphenidate — the odds of winning while doping improved by five percentage points, enough to “bring a player from world rank 5000 to 3500 (+35 Elo points),” according to the researchers.

“In sum, these results suggest that most players will benefit from [cognitive enhancement], in particular from modafinil and methylphenidate, while those who tend to be rather slow thinkers may perform worse in time-restricted games,” the authors wrote in the study. For people who don’t play chess, the study also suggests that these sorts of improvements are likely to translate to other complex tasks.

What does ‘smarter’ really mean?

The key to figuring out whether smart drugs — also known as nootropics — can boost brainpower is to determine exactly what sort of “smarts” are being improved. And in many ways, this recent study is one of the most informative investigations of nootropics’ effects.

Several prior tests of these drugs didn’t find any brain-boosting effects, but those were measuring mental deficiencies — looking at just one component of cognition and assessing when something wasn’t working. Those types of tests therefore aren’t great at measuring improvement in a way that mirrors real-life problem-solving.

“Rarely in life do we spend an entire day using a sole cognitive sub-domain – attention, for example,” Oxford researchers Ruairidh Battleday and Anna-Katherine Brem told Business Insider via email in a previous discussion about smart drugs. “Rather, we constantly plan, predict, and problem solve – all of which involve marshalling subdomains of cognition and integrating their output over varying tasks and difficulties.”

A test that just measures attention might only highlight the instances in which someone isn’t focusing, but might not be able to measure improvement beyond a certain point. 

“It is in this sense that complex tasks can approximate everyday functioning better than simple,” Battleday and Brem wrote.

The thinking required of a chess match is therefore a fascinating measure of whether or not a cognitive boost is occurring.

To find out how effective these drugs are as cognitive enhancers more generally, however, research is still needed. According to Battleday and Brem, scientists have not spent much time analysing how these drugs boost brainpower in healthy people, despite the fact that many people use them off-label for that purpose. But this study shows the drugs may indeed have a powerful effect on complex thought processes.

For chess, this means that the decision to start drug testing players is most likely a sound one. The rest of us might need to contemplate how we feel about pharmaceutical cognitive enhancement overall.

“[T]hese substances may be able to convert fast and shallow thinkers into deeper but somewhat slower thinkers,” the study authors wrote.

 

Post